Delayed voluntary exercise does not enhance cognitive performance after hippocampal injury: an investigation of differentially distributed exercise protocols

Article information

J Exerc Rehabil Vol. 12, No. 5, 401-412, October, 2016
Publication date (electronic) : 2016 October 31
doi : https://doi.org/10.12965/jer.1632680.340
1The Unit for Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
2Department of Drug Design and Pharmacology, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
*Corresponding author: Hana Malá, http://orcid.org/0000-0002-2910-372X, The Unit for Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen Oester Farimagsgade 2A, DK-1353 Copenhagen K, Denmark, Tel: +45-35324875, Fax: +45-35324802, E-mail: hana.mala@psy.ku.dk
Received 2016 July 14; Accepted 2016 September 05.

Abstract

Voluntary exercise has previously been shown to enhance cognitive recovery after acquired brain injury (ABI). The present study evaluated effects of two differentially distributed protocols of delayed, voluntary exercise on cognitive recovery using an allocentric place learning task in an 8-arm radial maze. Fifty-four Wistar rats were subjected to either bilateral transection of the fimbria-fornix (FF) or to sham surgery. Twenty-one days postinjury, the animals started exercising in running wheels either for 14 consecutive days (FF/exercise daily [ExD], sham/ExD) or every other day for 14 days (FF/exercise every second day [ExS], sham/ExS). Additional groups were given no exercise treatment (FF/not exercise [NE], sham/NE). Regardless of how exercise was distributed, we found no cognitively enhancing effects of exercise in the brain injured animals. Design and protocol factors possibly affecting the efficacy of post-ABI exercise are discussed.

INTRODUCTION

There is a growing interest in the effects of exercise on cognitive abilities and health. Clinical research has focused on cognitive effects of physical activity in healthy populations (Khan and Hillman, 2014; Smith et al., 2010), as well as in people with damage to the central nervous system (Beier et al., 2014; Öhman et al., 2014).

Cognitive impairment after acquired brain injury (ABI) is common and can compromise many mental abilities such as memory, attention, processing speed and executive function. Cognitive rehabilitation tends to focus on psychoeducation, training of specific cognitive domains, compensational strategies, and metacognitive awareness (Cicerone et al., 2011; Greenwald and Rigg, 2009). Physical exercise therapy on the other hand is a commonly employed rehabilitation tool to promote motor recovery after ABI (Greenwald and Rigg, 2009; Marshall et al., 2007). However, there are strong indications that exercise also improves cognitive functioning after ABI. Most data stems from preclinical models of brain injury (El-Tamawy et al., 2014; Grealy et al., 1999). Animal model-based research shows that exercise can promote cognitive recovery under certain circumstances, but findings are not consistent (Wogensen et al., 2015). Many animal model based studies have focused on the cognitive effects of exercise initiated early after injury (0–4 days postinjury). However, it is rare that brain injured patients presenting solely with cognitive problems receive exercise training as part of their rehabilitation plan in the early postinjury stages – if at all. Furthermore, cognitive dysfunctions can be difficult to diagnose in the early postinjury phases. To investigate whether exercise performed during later recovery phases can also promote cognitive recovery, we applied a delayed exercise paradigm (exercise starting 21 days postinjury) in the present study.

Presently, it is still unknown how, when and how much one should exercise to gain optimal effects on cognitive recovery. A scarcely investigated, but potentially important, factor is the temporal distribution of exercise – that is, whether one should exercise daily or whether there should be resting day(s) between exercise sessions. In most animal model studies, animals exercise every day (Wogensen et al., 2015). However, in a study by Cechetti et al. (2012) animals exposed to bilateral common carotid artery occlusion, were exercised three times a week for 12 weeks. This administration schedule improved learning, reference memory and working memory recovery in the brain injured animals. To our knowledge there are no studies directly comparing the cognitive effects of distributed versus continuous (daily) administered exercise after brain injury. As many patients in outpatient rehabilitation settings do not receive daily exercise training, it is relevant to address if there are differential effects of daily versus more distributed exercise. In this study, we therefore investigated the effects of voluntary exercise administered either daily or every other day for a total period of 14 days.

While ABI can cause tissue damage in many cerebral areas, hippocampal atrophy is common in both animal models and clinical studies of traumatic brain injury (Ariza et al., 2006). Related structures, such as the fornix, also show degeneration due to brain trauma (Tate and Bigler, 2000), and this process has been shown to continue into the later postinjury stages (Adnan et al., 2013). It is well-established that the hippocampus and its surrounding structures are involved in learning and memory processing (Opitz, 2014) as well as spatial representation (Moser et al., 2008). In the present study, we employed a partial model of brain injury, namely bilateral transection of the fimbria-fornix, which causes retrograde degeneration of the medial and dorsolateral septal nuclei and compromises the functionality of the hippocampus (Ginsberg and Martin, 1998; Li et al., 2005). This procedure is known to induce substantial learning and memory deficits in rats in a variety of cognitive tasks (Malá et al., 2005; Mogensen et al., 2004; Mogensen et al., 2007). Using this model of mechanical brain injury, we presently address the clinically important issue whether differentially distributed exercise protocols may be able to promote posttraumatic cognitive recovery when administered at a relatively late posttraumatic time point.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Subjects and experimental groups

Fifty-four experimentally naïve, male, Wistar rats weighing approx. 300 g at the beginning of the experiment served as experimental subjects. The animals were pair-housed in makrolon type 3 cages with elevated lids (allowing rearing in the cage) under controlled temperature (22°C±2°C) and humidity (50%±5%). A reversed 12-hr light/dark cycle was maintained in the animal quarters (lights on at 7:00 p.m.) and all experimental procedures took place during the animals’ dark phase. During the exercise period the animals were given commercial rat chow ad libitum. During behavioral procedures in the maze, the animals were fed once daily after training/testing and maintained at approximately 85% of their ad libitum body weights. Water was always available ad libitum.

The animals were randomly divided into six experimental groups:

  1. Sham surgery group not subjected to exercise (n=10, Sham/NE)

  2. Sham surgery group subjected to exercise daily (n=8, Sham/ExD)

  3. Sham surgery group subjected to exercise every second day (n=8, Sham/ExS)

  4. Fimbria-fornix transected group not subjected to exercise (n=12, FF/NE)

  5. Fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise daily (n=8, FF/ExD)

  6. Fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise every second day (n=8, FF/ExS)

The experiments were carried out in accordance with the guidelines of the Danish Animal Experimentation Act and the European Council Directive of 22 September 2010 (2010/63/EU). All efforts were made to minimize the number of animals used and their suffering according to the principle of the three Rs.

Voluntary exercise in running wheels

The voluntary exercise took place in ENV-042 activity wheels with attached modular holding cages (MED Associates Inc., St Albans, VT, USA). The holding cage—from which the animal could freely enter and exit the running wheel—was 17.8 cm in height, 16 cm wide and 20.3 cm in depth and equipped with bedding and unlimited access to water. The wheel was 35.6 cm in diameter and 11 cm in width with a free wheeling drag of approximately 12 g. The number of wheel revolutions per every 5 min was recorded by computer software (Ellegaard Systems A/S, Faaborg, Denmark). The mean number of revolutions was calculated for each period of 6 hr/day.

Rats had access to the running wheel apparatus during their dark period. Preoperatively – and before introduction to the 8-arm radial maze – the animals were habituated to the running wheels for 2 hr/day for three consecutive days. Postoperatively, the animals in the exercise groups were given access to the wheels either 6 hr daily for 14 consecutive days (a total of 14 exercise sessions) or 6 hr every second day for 14 days (a total of seven exercise sessions). All animals were weighed daily throughout the exercise period.

Allocentric place learning task in 8-arm radial maze

The behavioral procedures have been described elsewhere (Malá et al., 2005). All cognitive training and testing was performed in an open, darkly colored, 8-arm radial maze with 2.7 cm high walls and 9.2 cm wide corridors. The circular center of the maze had a diameter of 50 cm, from which eight arms, all 60 cm long, spread out with equal spacing. At the end of each arm a circular food well (diameter, 4.8 cm;, depth, 2.3 cm) contained reinforcements in the form of four 45-mg food pellets (Precision Food Pellets, Campden Instruments, Leics, UK). The maze was placed in the center of a well-lit room in which a multitude of two- and three-dimensional visual cues were available.

All training and testing of animals in the 8-arm radial maze were performed by trained experimenters who were blinded regarding the group affiliations of the animals. Preoperatively, the animals were habituated to the 8-arm radial maze for 2 days (16 min per day) and then shaped to promptly enter one of the arms of the maze for 19 consecutive days (15 trials a day). On the 43rd postoperative day, the animals resumed the behavioral procedures in the 8-arm radial maze. During the first three of these postoperative sessions, the animals were reshaped. On the fourth postoperative session, training and testing in an allocentric place learning task was initiated. All animals were given 15 trials per day for 28 consecutive days. One arm (defined according to its spatial location within the experimental room) was chosen as the goal arm. The remaining seven arms served as start arms and were selected in a randomized order. To limit the animals’ use of intramazial cues for the task solution, the maze was rotated daily before the beginning of testing. When entering the goal arm, the animal was allowed to reach the food well and consume four reinforcement pellets. If an incorrect arm was entered, the animal was picked up before reaching the food well. After each trial the animal was removed from the maze and put in a holding cage for approx. one minute. From each session, two parameters were recorded: ‘number of total errors’ (i.e., the number of times per session the animal did not choose the goal arm) and ‘number of distal errors’ (i.e., the number of times per session the animal chose neither the goal arm nor the two arms adjacent to the goal arm). Additionally, the parameters ‘summed errors’ (i.e., the summated number of total errors across all 28 sessions) and ‘summed distal errors’ (i.e., the summated number of distal errors across all 28 sessions) were calculated for each animal across all sessions. See Table 1 for an overview of the behavioral procedures.

Schematic overview of the behavioral procedures

Surgery

Surgery (which lasted approximately 30 min per animal) was performed with the aid of a surgical microscope under clean conditions. The animals were anesthetized with medetomidine (Dexdomitor, 0.37 mg/kg) and ketamine (Ketaminol, 62 mg/kg) and subsequently given atropine (0.3 mg/kg) – all via intraperitoneally injections. The animals’ eyes were lubricated with a protective salve (Neutral Ophtha) to prevent drying. The incision site was shaved, iodized and a topical analgesic (lidocain) was injected into the skin.

Bilateral transections of the fimbria-fornix were performed stereotaxically as described in detail elsewhere (Mogensen et al., 2004). To summarize, a hole was drilled into the skull 1.1 mm posteriorly to the bregma and 1.2 mm laterally to the sagittal suture, and a guiding cannula containing a wire-knife was lowered to a position 3.2 mm ventrally to the dura and extended 1.6 mm laterally. The wire-knife was further lowered to a position 5.0 mm ventrally to the dura and left for 1 min. The extended wire-knife was then returned to a position 3.2 mm ventrally to the dura. The knife was retracted into the guiding cannula and the instrument rotated 180°. Following extension, the wire-knife was again lowered to a position 5.0 mm ventrally to the dura and left for 1 min. The extended wire-knife was then returned to the position 3.2 mm ventrally to the dura. After retraction into the guiding cannula, the cannula was withdrawn from the brain tissue. The procedures were performed in both hemispheres. The sham operated animals were anaesthetized and their skulls exposed, however, the brain remained intact.

Prior to surgery, animals were habituated for 3 days to ingest Nutella (Ferrero, Alba, Italy), a hazelnut/chocolate spread. Immediately following surgery, all animals were given analgesic treatment with buprenorphine (Temgesic injection fluid, 0.03 mg/kg) and sterile saline (2 mL per animal) via subcutaneous injections. The animals were put in clean cages and kept under heating lamps until emerging from the anaesthesia. Analgesic treatment (Temgesic sublingual tablets, 0.03 mg/kg, crushed and mixed in Nutella) was administered twice daily and continued as long as needed. After cessation of the analgesic treatment, animals were given pure Nutella once a day for 2 days to avoid a sudden weight loss. Commercial rat chow soaked in water was provided to ease feeding during recovery and optimize water intake.

Histology

Upon finishing the behavioral testing, the animals were deeply anesthetized and transcardially perfused with 10% sucrose followed by a 4% buffered formaldehyde in saline solution. The brains were removed and stored at 4°C in 4% buffered formaldehyde in saline solution. Brain sections were cut horizontally on a vibratome at 50 μm and examined under a microscope (Leica DMD 108, Leica Microsystems, Wetzlar, Germany), to verify the locus of the lesion and to quantify the size of lesion. The proportion of fimbria-fornix fibres that remained intact was quantified using the microscope associated software. The transection was examined through its entire depth and measurements regarding two levels representing the dorsal and ventral edge of the FF fiber bundle were recorded: level 1 (dorsal: interaural, 6.40; bregma, −3.60) and level 2 (ventral: interaural, 5.72; bregma, −4.28) (Paxinos and Watson, 1986). The obtained values were averaged in the sham and lesioned animals, respectively, and percentage of the intact fibers in the lesioned animals was calculated relative to animals subjected to control sham surgery. The values regarding lesion sizes in the lesioned groups were subsequently analysed statistically.

Statistics analysis

All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics ver. 22.0 (IBM Co., Armonk, NY, USA).

The size of lesion in the three fimbria-fornix transected groups (FF/NE, FF/ExD, and FF/ExS) was estimated and compared using independent samples Kruskal-Wallis nonparametric test. Body weights during exercise were analysed by two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) to identify potential group differences on the first and last day of exercise administration. Paired-sample t-tests were used to compare within-group body weight changes between the first and last day of exercise.

Distances (m) run in the exercise wheels were analysed using a mixed design, repeated measures ANOVA over 14 exercise sessions (Sham/ExD and FF/ExD) and seven exercise sessions (Sham/ExS and FF/ExS). Total between-group mean running distances were analysed by two-way ANOVA.

Overall acquisition of the place learning task (‘summed total errors’ and ‘summed distal errors’) was analysed using two-way ANOVAs. To identify lesion effects and acquisition over time (‘number of total errors’ and ‘number of distal errors’), mixed design, repeated-measures ANOVAs were performed using the means obtained within each of seven groups of four sessions (sessions 1–4, 5–8, 9–12, 13–16, 17–20, 21–24, and 25–28). In case of violated sphericity, Greenhouse-Geisser correction was applied.

In order to identify the specific effects of the exercise procedures in the lesioned and control operated (sham) groups, separate mixed design, repeated-measures ANOVAs (using similar session intervals as above) were executed for sham and fimbria-fornix transected groups, respectively. All significances are two-tailed. Significance level is defined as P<0.05.

RESULTS

Anatomy

The histological examination revealed that in all of the lesioned animals, the fimbria-fornix rostral to the dorsal hippocampus was almost completely transected – only a minor portion of the fibres remained intact. There was no significant difference in the percentage of transected fibers in the three fimbria-fornix transected groups (FF/ExD, FF/ExS, and FF/NE) (P=0.340). In all animals, the fimbria and dorsal fornix were transected at the level of the ventral hippocampal commissure (anteroposterior distance to bregma, −1.30 mm to −1.40 mm). In a few animals, the damage extended ventrally into the subfornical organ, dorsally into the ventral part of corpus callosum, and laterally into the dorsomedial neostriatum. Fig. 1 schematically summarizes the extent of the lesion and shows that it did not differ across experimental groups.

Fig. 1

Schematic diagram of the fimbria-fornix transections. The gray area indicates the lesioned area. The outer line corresponds to area lesioned in at least one animal. The inner line corresponds to the area transected in all animals. Illustrations of the three groups that were subjected to the fimbria-fornix transection: the group not subjected to exercise (FF/NE), the group subjected to exercise daily (FF/ExD), and the group subjected to exercise every second day (FF/ExS) (for more information see ‘MATERIALS AND METHODS’ and ‘RESULTS’ sections). The diagrams show horizontal levels 6.40 and 5.72 in front of the interaural line (Paxinos and Watson, 1986). df, dorsal fornix; fi, fimbria; CPu, caudate putamen.

Body weights during exercise

A two-way ANOVA of body weights on the first day of exercise revealed no effect of surgery (F [1, 48]=0.210, P=0.649), exercise (F [2, 48]=0.372, P=0.691) or surgery×exercise (F [2, 48]=0.318, P=0.729). This was also the case on the last day of exercise (surgery: F [1, 48]=0.022, P=0.883; exercise: F [2, 48]=2.441, P= 0.098; surgery×exercise: F [2,48]=0.763, P=0.472).

Paired-sample t-tests comparing first and last day body weights within each individual group revealed weight increases from start to end in Sham/NE (P<0.001); Sham/ExS (P<0.005); FF/NE (P< 0.001), and FF/ExS (P<0.01), and no weight differences in Sham/ExD and FF/ExD (P=0.422 and P=0.344, respectively) (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2

Body weights (g) on the first day (green bars) and last day (purple bars) of exercise. All values are given as means with standard error of the mean. Sham/NE, sham surgery group not subjected to exercise; Sham/ExD, sham surgery group subjected to exercise daily; Sham/ExS, sham surgery group subjected to exercise every second day; FF/NE, fimbria-fornix transected group not subjected to exercise; FF/ExD, fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise daily; FF/ExS, fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise every second day.

Running wheel exercise

Mean running distances across exercise sessions were 264.7 m (Sham/ExD); 367.6 m (FF/ExD); 366.9 m (Sham/ExS); and 408.0 m (FF/ExS).

A mixed design, repeated measures ANOVA of running distances of the groups allowed access to running wheels for 14 consecutive days (Sham/ExD and FF/ExD) showed a significant effect of time (F [3.607, 50.496]=2.892, P<0.05), i.e., increased running over time in both groups. No time×surgery effect (F [3.607, 50.496]=1.258, P=0.299) was found (Fig. 3A).

Fig. 3

(A) Running distances (m) of groups exercised daily for a total of 14 sessions. Open circle: Sham/ExD; black circle: FF/ExD. (B) Running distances (m) of groups exercised every second day for a total of 7 sessions. Open circle: Sham/ExS; black circle: FF/ExS. (C) Total running distances (m) of all exercised groups. Values are given as means with standard error of the mean. Sham/ExD, sham surgery group subjected to exercise daily; Sham/ExS, sham surgery group subjected to exercise every second day; FF/ExD, fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise daily; FF/ExS, fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise every second day.

A mixed design, repeated measures ANOVA of running distances in the groups running every other day (Sham/ExS and FF/ExS) showed no effects of time (F [2.934, 41.070]=1.996, P=0.131) or time × surgery (F [2.934, 41.070]=0.509, P=0.674) (Fig. 3B).

Subsequent two-way ANOVA of mean total distances comparing all exercise groups showed no effect of either surgery (F [1, 28]= 0.893, P=0.353), exercise (F [1, 28]=0.912, P=0.348), or surgery×exercise (F [1, 28]=0.167, P=0.686) (Fig. 3C).

Behavior

A two-way ANOVA investigating ‘summed total errors’ revealed a significant effect of surgery (F [1, 48]=130.970, P<0.001), indicating significantly fewer errors in the sham-groups compared to the FF-groups (Fig. 4A). There was no effect of exercise (F [2, 48]=1.177, P=0.317) or surgery×exercise (F [2, 48]=1.710, P=0.192). The same analysis using ‘summed distal errors’ as the dependent variable produced a similar picture with a clear effect of surgery, but no other significant effects (surgery: F [1, 48]=87.171, P<0.001; exercise: F [2, 48]=0.639, P=0.532; surgery×exercise: F [2, 48]=1.419, P=0.252) (Fig. 4B). This illustrates, that the lesioned animals performed significantly worse on both investigated parameters than the sham-operated animals regardless of exercise treatment.

Fig. 4

(A) Summed total errors across the 28 acquisition sessions. (B) Summed distal errors across the 28 acquisition sessions. All values are given as means with standard error of the mean. Sham/NE, sham surgery group not subjected to exercise; Sham/ExD, sham surgery group subjected to exercise daily; Sham/ExS, sham surgery group subjected to exercise every second day; FF/NE, fimbria-fornix transected group not subjected to exercise; FF/ExD, fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise daily; FF/ExS, fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise every second day.

A mixed design, repeated measures ANOVA of ‘number of total errors’ of every four-session interval revealed a significant effect of time (F [2.645, 126.948]=310.791, P<0.001), indicating learning in all groups, as well as a significant effect of time×surgery (F [2.645, 126.948]=45.341, P<0.001), indicating superior learning over time in the nonlesioned (sham) groups. No interaction effects of time×exercise (F [5.289, 126.948]=0.987, P=0.431) or time×exercise×surgery (F [5.289, 126.948]=1.133, P=0.347) were found. Between-subjects analysis showed a clear lesion effect (F [1, 48]=130.970, P<0.001), however, no effect of exercise (F [2, 48]=1.177, P=0.317) or surgery×exercise interaction effect (F [2, 48]=1.710, P=0.192). Using the same analysis with ‘number of distal errors’ as the dependent variable revealed a significant effect of time (F [2.884, 138.435]=259.074, P<0.001) and time×surgery (F [2.884, 138.435]=32.987, P<0.001), but no interaction effects regarding time×exercise (F [5.768, 138.435]=1.029, P= 0.408) or time×surgery×exercise (F [5.768, 138.435]=0.413, P= 0.863). Between-subjects analysis demonstrated an effect of surgery (F [1, 48]=87.171, P<0.001), but no effects of exercise or surgery ×exercise (F [2, 48]=0.639, P=0.532 and F [2, 48]=1.419, P= 0.252, respectively).

In order to investigate possible separate effects of exercise in the intact and brain injured animals, subsequent mixed design, repeated measures ANOVAs (regarding ‘number of total errors’ and ‘number of distal errors’) were performed in the lesioned and sham groups, respectively. Analysing ‘number of total errors’ in the three lesioned groups (FF/NE, FF/ExD, and FF/ExS) revealed a time effect (F [2.593, 64.819]=118.532, P<0.001), but no time x exercise effect (F [5.185, 64.819]=1.150, P=0.344). Between-subject analysis revealed no significant differences between exercise groups (F [2, 25]=1.537; P=0.235), indicating that the exercise intervention did not affect task acquisition. Regarding ‘number of distal errors’ the same analysis also showed an effect of time (F [2.791, 69.768]=110.537, P<0.001), but not of time×exercise (F [5.581, 69.768]=0.498, P=0.796). There were no significant between-groups differences (F [2, 25]=1.056; P=0.363), again indicating no effects of the exercise treatment on the number of distal errors in the lesioned groups.

Analysis of the performance of the sham-operated groups (Sham/NE, Sham/ExD, and Sham/ExS) showed a significant effect of time (F [1.540, 35.416]=346.740, P<0.001) regarding ‘number of total errors’. There was neither a significant time×exercise effect (F [3.080, 35.416]=0.959, P=0.425) nor significant effect of the between-subjects analysis (F [2, 23]=1.676, P=0.209). Regarding ‘number of distal errors’, the overall pattern was the same, showing an effect of time (F [1.865, 42.892]=308.957, P<0.001), but no interaction effect (F [3.730, 42.892]=1.733, P=0.164) or between-subjects effect (F [2, 23]=1.711, P=0.203).

DISCUSSION

In the current study we found that voluntary exercise initiated 21 days after bilateral transection of the fimbria-fornix did not enhance cognitive recovery – neither when the exercise was carried out daily for 14 days nor when it was administered every other day for 14 days (Fig. 5). This result is surprising, as other studies have reported cognitively enhancing effects of voluntary exercise paradigms administered in the late stages after brain injury (Gram et al., 2016; Piao et al., 2013; Winocur et al., 2012; Wong-Goodrich et al., 2010). Such results have, however, not been obtained in all cases (Clark et al., 2008).

Fig. 5

(A) Number of total errors during acquisition of the allocentric place learning task. (B) Number of distal errors during acquisition of the allocentric place learning task. Open circle: Sham/NE, open triangle: Sham/ExD, open square: Sham/ExS, black circle: FF/NE, black triangle: FF/ExD, black square: FF/ExS. All values are given as means with standard error of the mean. Sham/NE, sham surgery group not subjected to exercise; Sham/ExD, sham surgery group subjected to exercise daily; Sham/ExS, sham surgery group subjected to exercise every second day; FF/NE, fimbria-fornix transected group not subjected to exercise; FF/ExD, fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise daily; FF/ExS, fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise every second day.

Several factors limit our knowledge regarding the effects of exercise on cognitive recovery in animals subjected to ABI. Firstly, animal model-based research on the subject in general – and studies investigating effects of late exercise (i.e., three weeks or more after injury) in particular – is scarce and methodologically diverse. Secondly, animal studies on effects of voluntary exercise starting in the early stages after ABI (i.e., during the first three postinjury weeks) show a somewhat mixed picture. Some studies find positive effects on cognitive recovery, some report no changes and some find detrimental effects (Wogensen et al., 2015). Research on the effects of forced exercise paradigms after ABI generally find positive effects of early initiated exercise (Itoh et al., 2011, Kim et al., 2010, Sim et al., 2004). Given this picture, one may be tempted to conclude that exercise most effectively promotes cognitive recovery when administered in the early postinjury stages. However, the available data regarding effects of late exercise on cognitive recovery after brain injury is limited to only a few studies (Wogensen et al., 2015). A delayed administration of exercise most closely resembles the clinical practice, where exercise is sometimes used as a supportive intervention in comprehensive rehabilitation programs in the postacute phase after ABI. More research is therefore needed to elucidate the effects of delayed exercise on parameters of cognitive recovery.

Out of the limited number of studies investigating the cognitive recovery effects of exercise administered at later time points after ABI, most use radiation exposure models (Clark et al., 2008; Winocur et al., 2012; Wong-Goodrich et al., 2010). It is presently unclear whether outcomes of exercise intervention in irradiation models can be directly compared to other models of ABI. Cancer studies have shown that therapeutic doses of irradiation can cause neurotoxic effects – especially affecting hippocampal neurogenesis, cell proliferation, differentiation and apoptosis (Dietrich et al., 2008; Monje, 2008). Stroke or traumatic brain injuries generally produce more widespread and diffuse cerebral pathologies (Bramlett and Dietrich, 2004), although hippocampal atrophy is a common finding in human survivors of traumatic brain injury (Ariza et al., 2006; Green et al., 2014; Tomaiuolo et al., 2004). The injury model used in the present study (fimbria-fornix transection) is a focal mechanical injury specifically targeting hippocampal functioning. On one hand, there is a basis for comparison with irradiation-induced brain injury models as both models specifically affect hippocampal processes. On the other hand, the injury infliction as well as the processes known to be affected by the transection differ from those of irradiation-associated injury. As part of the septo-hippocampal pathway, the fimbria-fornix relays important cholinergic input to the hippocampus, which is significantly reduced by the transection (Ayala-Grosso et al., 2004). The transection has also been shown to affect hippocampal long-term potentiation (Li et al., 2005). The fact that the positive effects of exercise reported by some of the irradiation studies (for references see above) are not replicated in our study may therefore be attributable to differences in model pathologies.

Other potentially important factors could also have contributed to the lack of exercise effects in the current study: running distance as well as intensity and duration of exercise. The most widely used measure of activity in voluntary exercise paradigms is the average distance run over sessions. In the current study, the lesioned animals ran 368 m (FF/ExD) and 408 m (FF/ExS) on average per exercise session. These distances are comparable to distances found in other studies showing recovery enhancing effects after late voluntary exercise (Gram et al., 2016; Piao et al., 2013; Wong-Goodrich et al., 2010). Positive effects of post-ABI voluntary exercise on cognitive recovery have been found in animals running between approximately 100 m and (more than) 8 km a day (Wogensen et al., 2015), showing that both short and long distances can promote recovery. In our study we see no such recovery promoting effects. This could mean that either the animals in our study ran too short a distance to promote recovery or the average running distances are not a reliable measures of exercise efficiency. Moreover, it is known that different mice and rat stocks/strains exhibit different patterns of spontaneous activity in the running wheel with some types running longer distances than others (Coletti et al., 2013; Johnson and Mitchell, 2003; Lightfoot et al., 2004). For instance, investigating spontaneous running wheel activity in 14 different mouse strains, Lightfoot et al. (2004) found significant variations not only in running distances, but also in durations and velocity of running. This suggests that the genetic background of animals can influence the pattern and intensity with which they exercise and that the choice of rodent strain/stock might therefore influence recovery outcome in voluntary running paradigms. This might also contribute to the heterogeneous results reported by studies in this field. As none of the studies reporting cognition enhancing outcomes after voluntary exercise in brain injured rats used the Wistar stock, one may speculate whether the animals in our study had a sufficient spontaneous activity level to promote cognitive recovery.

Additionally, voluntary exercise paradigms do not allow experimental control of running intensity. Studies using forced exercise paradigms indicate that intensity plays a role for the efficacy of exercise, with low-to-moderate intensity leading to enhanced cognitive recovery (Shen et al., 2013; Shih et al., 2013; Shimada et al., 2013). However, this is not a homogenous finding, as more high intensity exercise has also been reported to induce positive effects on cognitive recovery (Cechetti et al., 2012; Itoh et al., 2011), while low intensity exercise in some cases has been without effect (Chen et al., 2006). These findings suggest that exercise intensity is an important factor for promotion of recovery, and that the lack of intensity control associated with voluntary exercise could be an intrinsic weakness of the paradigm. Furthermore, exercise perseverance and pauses in activity can also influence exercise efficacy. Most forced exercise paradigms demand uninterrupted running at a specific pace, often for 30 min (Wogensen et al., 2015). However, such running continuity may not be obtained when using voluntary exercise. Ploughman et al. (2007) found that animals in nonmotorized activity wheels ran and stopped frequently. Running pattern samplings from the present study has revealed a similar picture with intermittent periods of running and pausing (Fig. 6). Furthermore, the samplings indicate that the length of the pauses increases during the exercise period (i.e., from day 1 through day 7 to day 14). Daily running distances, however, do not decrease (Fig. 3A, B). Thus, it appears that the pattern of running changes during the exercise period (Fig. 6). As a consequence of this changed running pattern, the animals in our set-up may not have reached the level of intensity or running continuity necessary to promote cognitive recovery.

Fig. 6

Running and pausing pattern examples of animals in the four exercise groups (FF/ExD, FF/ExS, Sham/ExD, Sham/ExS) at the beginning and end of the exercise period. Measurements were obtained every 5 min (indicated by dots) for 6 hr. Pausing was defined as <5 wheel rotations per every 5 min measuring point, running was defined as ≥5.1 wheel rotations per every 5 min measuring point. Sham/ExD, sham surgery group subjected to exercise daily; Sham/ExS, sham surgery group subjected to exercise every second day; FF/ExD, fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise daily; FF/ExS, fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise every second day.

A related question has to do with the duration of the intervention – both per session and the number of exercise sessions. Many voluntary exercise paradigms allow 24-hr access to running wheels in the home cages (Griesbach et al., 2009; Winocur et al., 2012). However, this often entails that the animals are single housed. Single housing has been shown to decrease neurogenesis and the levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor and increase stress-susceptibility in intact animals (Berry et al., 2012; Leasure and Decker, 2009; Stranahan et al., 2006). In the present study we allowed the animals 6 hr of access to the exercise apparatus (during their active phase) and then returned the animal to its home cage with its cage mate. To our knowledge, it is currently unresolved whether and how single housing affects cognitive recovery in brain injured animals. Future research should also apply designs that control for social deprivation as a confounding factor in the study of exercise effects on cognitive recovery. Moreover, total duration of the exercise may also play a role. Effects on cognitive recovery have been found in voluntary paradigms administering exercise for 1 week up to approximately 6-week duration (Wogensen et al., 2015). The 14 days of exercise administered in this study should therefore fall within the potentially therapeutic range. However, as already mentioned, other variables, such as injury, timing and running intensity, are likely to be responsible for the lack of therapeutic effects in the current set-up.

Finally, it is unknown whether the applied intervention was too general to promote recovery or whether recovery of the applied cognitive task would require a more task-specific training. Several studies in healthy animals have suggested that task-specific (i.e., learning based) limb training is more effective in producing plastic changes in the motor cortex than a more general activation through mere limb use (Kleim et al., 1998). Using rats subjected to cerebral ischemia, Biernaskie and Corbett (2001) showed that a combination of general enriched environment and specific skilled motor training resulted in better motor recovery and increased neuronal plasticity in the motor cortex than animals kept in standard housing conditions. This opens the question of whether exercise would be more effective as a postinjury intervention if it has a concomitant task learning aspect. Further research is needed to elucidate if more generally activating exercise protocols (i.e., using more than one limb) with a concomitant task-specific learning element could promote recovery—including cognitive recovery—after brain injury.

Thus, we are still in the early stages of elucidating the effects of late voluntary exercise on cognitive recovery after ABI. While research into the cognitive effects of exercise in both preclinical and clinical populations show promise, research methods and results are still highly diverse. Factors such as type of injury, animal species, timing and dose of the exercise intervention appear to be central for the effects on cognitive outcome. Future research in this area would benefit from more methodologically homogenous studies further investigating these parameters.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The present study was supported by a grant from the Danish Council for Independent Research. The authors wish to thank Martin Blumensaadt Bach, Varvara Bashkirova, Jennifer Cooper, Patricia Fernández, Siri Mehlsen, Laura Obel, Jeppe Olsen, and Nikolaj Daugaard Sørensen for their assistance with the behavioral testing.

Notes

CONFLICTS OF INTEREST

The authors have nothing to disclose.

References

Adnan, A., Crawley, A., Mikulis, D., Moscovitch, M., Colella, B., & Green, R.Moderate-severe traumatic brain injury causes delayed loss of white matter integrity: evidence of fornix deterioration in the chronic stage of injury. Brain Inj, (2013). 27, 1415–1422.
Ariza, M., Serra-Grabulosa, J.M., Junqué, C., Ramírez, B., Mataró, M., Poca, A., Bargalló, N., & Sahuquillo, J.Hippocampal head atrophy after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia, (2006). 44, 1956–1961.
Ayala-Grosso, C., Tam, J., Xanthoudakis, S., Bureau, Y., Roy, S., Nicholson, D.W., & Robertson, G.S.Effects of fimbria-fornix transection on calpain and choline acetyl transferase activities in the septohippocampal pathway. Neuroscience, (2004). 126, 927–940.
Beier, M., Bombardier, C.H., Hartoonian, N., Motl, R.W., & Kraft, G.H.Improved physical fitness correlates with improved cognition in multiple sclerosis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil, (2014). 95, 1328–1334.
Berry, A., Bellisario, V., Capoccia, S., Tirassa, P., Calza, A., Alleva, E., & Cirulli, F.Social deprivation stress is a triggering factor for the emergence of anxiety- and depression-like behaviours and leads to reduced brain BDNF levels in C57BL/6J mice. Psychoneuroendocrinology, (2012). 37, 762–772.
Biernaskie, J., & Corbett, D.Enriched rehabilitative training promotes improved forelimb motor function and enhanced dendritic growth after focal ischemic injury. J Neurosci, (2001). 21, 5272–5280.
Bramlett, H.M., & Dietrich, W.D.Pathophysiology of cerebral ischemia and brain trauma: similarities and differences. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab, (2004). 24, 133–150.
Cechetti, F., Worm, P.V., Elsner, V.R., Bertoldi, K., Sanches, E., Ben, J., Siqueira, I.R., & Netto, C.A.Forced treadmill exercise prevents oxidative stress and memory deficits following chronic cerebral hypoperfusion in the rat. Neurobiol Learn Mem, (2012). 97, 90–96.
Chen, L., Gong, S., Shan, L.D., Xu, W.P., Zhang, Y.J., Guo, S.Y., Hisamitsu, T., Yin, Q.Z., & Jiang, X.H.Effects of exercise on neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus and ability of learning and memory after hippocampus lesion in adult rats. Neurosci Bull, (2006). 22, 1–6.
Cicerone, K.D., Langenbahn, D.M., Braden, C., Malec, J.F., Kalmar, K., Fraas, M., Felicetti, T., Laatsch, L., Harley, J.P., Bergquist, T., Azulay, J., Cantor, J., & Ashman, T.Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: updated review of the literature from 2003 through 2008. Arch Phys Med Rehabil, (2011). 92, 519–530.
Clark, P.J., Brzezinska, W.J., Thomas, M.W., Ryzhenko, N.A., Toshkov, S.A., & Rhodes, J.S.Intact neurogenesis is required for benefits of exercise on spatial memory but not motor performance or contextual fear conditioning in C57BL/6J mice. Neuroscience, (2008). 155, 1048–1058.
Coletti, D., Berardi, E., Aulino, P., Rossi, E., Moresi, V., Li, Z., & Adamo, S.Substrains of inbred mice differ in their physical activity as a behavior. ScientificWorldJournal, (2013). 2013, 237260.
Dietrich, J., Monje, M., Wefel, J., & Meyers, C.Clinical patterns and biological correlates of cognitive dysfunction associated with cancer therapy. Oncologist, (2008). 13, 1285–1295.
El-Tamawy, M.S., Abd-Allah, F., Ahmed, S.M., Darwish, M.H., & Khalifa, H.A.Aerobic exercises enhance cognitive functions and brain derived neurotrophic factor in ischemic stroke patients. NeuroRehabilitation, (2014). 34, 209–213.
Ginsberg, S.D., & Martin, L.J.Ultrastructural analysis of the progression of neurodegeneration in the septum following fimbria-fornix transection. Neuroscience, (1998). 86, 1259–1272.
Gram, M.G., Wogensen, E., Moseholm, K., Mogensen, J., & Malá, H.Exercise-induced improvement in cognitive performance after fimbria-fornix transection depends on the timing of exercise administration. Brain Res Bull, (2016). 125, 117–126.
Grealy, M.A., Johnson, D.A., & Rushton, S.K.Improving cognitive function after brain injury: the use of exercise and virtual reality. Arch Phys Med Rehabil, (1999). 80, 661–667.
Green, R.E., Colella, B., Maller, J.J., Bayley, M., Glazer, J., & Mikulis, D.J.Scale and pattern of atrophy in the chronic stages of moderate-severe TBI. Front Hum Neurosci, (2014). 8, 67.
Greenwald, B.D., & Rigg, J.L.Neurorehabilitation in traumatic brain injury: does it make a difference? Mt Sinai J Med, (2009). 76, 182–189.
Griesbach, G.S., Hovda, D.A., & Gomez-Pinilla, F.Exercise-induced improvement in cognitive performance after traumatic brain injury in rats is dependent on BDNF activation. Brain Res, (2009). 1288, 105–115.
Itoh, T., Imano, M., Nishida, S., Tsubaki, M., Hashimoto, S., Ito, A., & Satou, T.Exercise inhibits neuronal apoptosis and improves cerebral function following rat traumatic brain injury. J Neural Transm (Vienna), (2011). 118, 1263–1272.
Johnson, R.A., & Mitchell, G.S.Exercise-induced changes in hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor and neurotrophin-3: effects of rat strain. Brain Res, (2003). 983, 108–114.
Khan, N.A., & Hillman, C.H.The relation of childhood physical activity and aerobic fitness to brain function and cognition: a review. Pediatr Exerc Sci, (2014). 26, 138–146.
Kim, D.H., Ko, I.G., Kim, B.K., Kim, T.W., Kim, S.E., Shin, M.S., Kim, C.J., Kim, H., Kim, K.M., & Baek, S.S.Treadmill exercise inhibits traumatic brain injury-induced hippocampal apoptosis. Physiol Behav, (2010). 101, 660–665.
Kleim, J.A., Barbay, S., & Nudo, R.J.Functional reorganization of the rat motor cortex following motor skill learning. J Neurophysiol, (1998). 80, 3321–3325.
Leasure, J.L., & Decker, L.Social isolation prevents exercise-induced proliferation of hippocampal progenitor cells in female rats. Hippocampus, (2009). 19, 907–912.
Li, C., Maier, D.L., Cross, B., Doherty, J.J., & Christian, E.P.Fimbria-fornix lesions compromise the induction of long-term potentiation at the Schaffer collateral-CA1 synapse in the rat in vivo. J Neurophysiol, (2005). 93, 3001–3006.
Lightfoot, J.T., Turner, M.J., Daves, M., Vordermark, A., & Kleeberger, S.R.Genetic influence on daily wheel running activity level. Physiol Genomics, (2004). 19, 270–276.
Malá, H., Alsina, C.G., Madsen, K.S., Sibbesen, El, Stick, H., & Mogensen, J.Erythropoietin improves place learning in an 8-arm radial maze in fimbria-fornix transected rats. Neural Plast, (2005). 12, 329–340.
Marshall, S., Teasell, R., Bayona, N., Lippert, C., Chundamala, J., Villamere, J., Mackie, D., Cullen, N., & Bayley, M.Motor impairment rehabilitation post acquired brain injury. Brain Inj, (2007). 21, 133–160.
Mogensen, J., Hjortkjaer, J., Ibervang, K.L., Stedal, K., & Malá, H.Prefrontal cortex and hippocampus in posttraumatic functional recovery: spatial delayed alternation by rats subjected to transection of the fimbria-fornix and/or ablation of the prefrontal cortex. Brain Res Bull, (2007). 73, 86–95.
Mogensen, J., Miskowiak, K., Sørensen, T.A., Lind, C.T., Olsen, N.V., Springborg, J.B., & Malá, H.Erythropoietin improves place learning in fimbria-fornix-transected rats and modifies the search pattern of normal rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, (2004). 77, 381–390.
Monje, M.Cranial radiation therapy and damage to hippocampal neurogenesis. Dev Disabil Res Rev, (2008). 14, 238–242.
Moser, E.I., Kropff, E., & Moser, M.B.Place cells, grid cells, and the brain’s spatial representation system. Annu Rev Neurosci, (2008). 31, 69–89.
Öhman, H., Savikko, N., Strandberg, T.E., & Pitkälä, K.H.Effect of physical exercise on cognitive performance in older adults with mild cognitive impairment or dementia: a systematic review. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord, (2014). 38, 347–365.
Opitz, B.Memory function and the hippocampus. Front Neurol Neurosci, (2014). 34, 51–59.
Paxinos, G., & Watson, C.The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. Sydney: Academic Press; (1986.
Piao, C.S., Stoica, B.A., Wu, J., Sabirzhanov, B., Zhao, Z., Cabatbat, R., Loane, D.J., & Faden, A.I.Late exercise reduces neuroinflammation and cognitive dysfunction after traumatic brain injury. Neurobiol Dis, (2013). 54, 252–263.
Ploughman, M., Granter-Button, S., Chernenko, G., Attwood, Z., Tucker, B.A., Mearow, K.M., & Corbett, D.Exercise intensity influences the temporal profile of growth factors involved in neuronal plasticity following focal ischemia. Brain Res, (2007). 1150, 207–216.
Shen, X., Li, A., Zhang, Y., Dong, X., Shan, T., Wu, Y., Jia, J., & Hu, Y.The effect of different intensities of treadmill exercise on cognitive function deficit following a severe controlled cortical impact in rats. Int J Mol Sci, (2013). 14, 21598–21612.
Shih, P.C., Yang, Y.R., & Wang, R.Y.Effects of exercise intensity on spatial memory performance and hippocampal synaptic plasticity in transient brain ischemic rats. PLoS One, (2013). 8, e78163.
Shimada, H., Hamakawa, M., Ishida, A., Tamakoshi, K., Nakashima, H., & Ishida, K.Low-speed treadmill running exercise improves memory function after transient middle cerebral artery occlusion in rats. Behav Brain Res, (2013). 243, 21–27.
Sim, Y.J., Kim, S.S., Kim, J.Y., Shin, M.S., & Kim, C.J.Treadmill exercise improves short-term memory by suppressing ischemia-induced apoptosis of neuronal cells in gerbils. Neurosci Lett, (2004). 372, 256–261.
Smith, P.J., Blumenthal, J.A., Hoffman, B.M., Cooper, H., Strauman, T.A., Welsh-Bohmer, K., Browndyke, J.N., & Sherwood, A.Aerobic exercise and neurocognitive performance: a meta-analytic review of randomized controlled trials. Psychosom Med, (2010). 72, 239–252.
Stranahan, A.M., Khalil, D., & Gould, E.Social isolation delays the positive effects of running on adult neurogenesis. Nat Neurosci, (2006). 9, 526–533.
Tate, D.F., & Bigler, E.D.Fornix and hippocampal atrophy in traumatic brain injury. Learn Mem, (2000). 7, 442–446.
Tomaiuolo, F., Carlesimo, G.A., Di Paola, M., Petrides, M., Fera, F., Bonanni, R., Formisano, R., Pasqualetti, P., & Caltagirone, C.Gross morphology and morphometric sequelae in the hippocampus, fornix, and corpus callosum of patients with severe non-missile traumatic brain injury without macroscopically detectable lesions: a T1 weighted MRI study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry, (2004). 75, 1314–1322.
Winocur, G., Becker, S., Luu, P., Rosenzweig, S., & Wojtowicz, J.M.Adult hippocampal neurogenesis and memory interference. Behav Brain Res, (2012). 227, 464–469.
Wogensen, E., Malá, H., & Mogensen, J.The effects of exercise on cognitive recovery after acquired brain injury in animal models: a systematic review. Neural Plast, (2015). 2015, 830871.
Wong-Goodrich, S.J., Pfau, M.L., Flores, C.T., Fraser, J.A., Williams, C.L., & Jones, L.W.Voluntary running prevents progressive memory decline and increases adult hippocampal neurogenesis and growth factor expression after whole-brain irradiation. Cancer Res, (2010). 70, 9329–9338.

Article information Continued

Fig. 1

Schematic diagram of the fimbria-fornix transections. The gray area indicates the lesioned area. The outer line corresponds to area lesioned in at least one animal. The inner line corresponds to the area transected in all animals. Illustrations of the three groups that were subjected to the fimbria-fornix transection: the group not subjected to exercise (FF/NE), the group subjected to exercise daily (FF/ExD), and the group subjected to exercise every second day (FF/ExS) (for more information see ‘MATERIALS AND METHODS’ and ‘RESULTS’ sections). The diagrams show horizontal levels 6.40 and 5.72 in front of the interaural line (Paxinos and Watson, 1986). df, dorsal fornix; fi, fimbria; CPu, caudate putamen.

Fig. 2

Body weights (g) on the first day (green bars) and last day (purple bars) of exercise. All values are given as means with standard error of the mean. Sham/NE, sham surgery group not subjected to exercise; Sham/ExD, sham surgery group subjected to exercise daily; Sham/ExS, sham surgery group subjected to exercise every second day; FF/NE, fimbria-fornix transected group not subjected to exercise; FF/ExD, fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise daily; FF/ExS, fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise every second day.

Fig. 3

(A) Running distances (m) of groups exercised daily for a total of 14 sessions. Open circle: Sham/ExD; black circle: FF/ExD. (B) Running distances (m) of groups exercised every second day for a total of 7 sessions. Open circle: Sham/ExS; black circle: FF/ExS. (C) Total running distances (m) of all exercised groups. Values are given as means with standard error of the mean. Sham/ExD, sham surgery group subjected to exercise daily; Sham/ExS, sham surgery group subjected to exercise every second day; FF/ExD, fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise daily; FF/ExS, fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise every second day.

Fig. 4

(A) Summed total errors across the 28 acquisition sessions. (B) Summed distal errors across the 28 acquisition sessions. All values are given as means with standard error of the mean. Sham/NE, sham surgery group not subjected to exercise; Sham/ExD, sham surgery group subjected to exercise daily; Sham/ExS, sham surgery group subjected to exercise every second day; FF/NE, fimbria-fornix transected group not subjected to exercise; FF/ExD, fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise daily; FF/ExS, fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise every second day.

Fig. 5

(A) Number of total errors during acquisition of the allocentric place learning task. (B) Number of distal errors during acquisition of the allocentric place learning task. Open circle: Sham/NE, open triangle: Sham/ExD, open square: Sham/ExS, black circle: FF/NE, black triangle: FF/ExD, black square: FF/ExS. All values are given as means with standard error of the mean. Sham/NE, sham surgery group not subjected to exercise; Sham/ExD, sham surgery group subjected to exercise daily; Sham/ExS, sham surgery group subjected to exercise every second day; FF/NE, fimbria-fornix transected group not subjected to exercise; FF/ExD, fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise daily; FF/ExS, fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise every second day.

Fig. 6

Running and pausing pattern examples of animals in the four exercise groups (FF/ExD, FF/ExS, Sham/ExD, Sham/ExS) at the beginning and end of the exercise period. Measurements were obtained every 5 min (indicated by dots) for 6 hr. Pausing was defined as <5 wheel rotations per every 5 min measuring point, running was defined as ≥5.1 wheel rotations per every 5 min measuring point. Sham/ExD, sham surgery group subjected to exercise daily; Sham/ExS, sham surgery group subjected to exercise every second day; FF/ExD, fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise daily; FF/ExS, fimbria-fornix transected group subjected to exercise every second day.

Table 1

Schematic overview of the behavioral procedures

Experimental phase Duration Experimental group Feeding
Habituation to exercise apparatus 3 Days All exercise groups Ad libitum
Habituation to maze 2 Days All Food deprivation
Shaping 19 Days All Food deprivation
Surgery 30 Min All Ad libitum
Postsurgery break 21 Days All Ad libitum
Exercise 14 Days (either daily or bi-daily) Sham or FF: ExD, ExS Ad libitum
Reshaping 3 Days All Food deprivation
Place learning task 28 Days All Food deprivation

FF, fimbria-fornix; ExD, exercise daily; ExS, exercise every second day.